(Students frequently rise
to the challenge when teachers raise the bar. Give them something to stretch
their minds with and students will embrace it with vigor and purpose.
Elementary statistics is a
transfer course at California’s community colleges for the CSU/UC systems. A
major part of this course is probability, the workhorse of statistics. What if
community college students were asked to read Richard Feynman’s lecture on
probability? What would they make of it? Richard Feynman
(1918-1988) won the Nobel Prize for physics in 1965 for his seminal
contributions to quantum electrodynamics. He is celebrated for his physical
insights and for his ability to clarify complex concepts for the general
audience. His fame grew when he gave a series of lectures on physics at the
California Institute of Technology for undergraduates from 1961-1963 that
became the three-volume “The Feynman Lectures on Physics.” More than four
decades later, the “Red Books” are still being read and still continue to
inspire. The lectures are now available online. His lecture on probability challenged several
community college students who found it fascinating and engrossing.)
Josh
found the discussion on the uncertainty principle most interesting, Nature is probabilistic rather than
deterministic, reasons enough for Josh to focus on mastering probability.
Feynman says that “the ideas of probability are certainly useful in describing
the behavior of the 1022 or so molecules in a sample of a gas, for
it is clearly impractical even to attempt to write down the position or
velocity of each molecule.” Hence his conclusion: “We now believe that the
ideas of probability are essential to a description of atomic happenings,” and
“our most precise description of nature must be in terms of probabilities.”
Most statistics texts introduce probability through flipping coins or rolling
dice that leaves students cold. For Josh, a connection between probability and
nature at its most fundamental level is a compelling argument for understanding
and working with probability.
Reese found the lecture interesting
but hard to follow. He gets it, though, when Feynman says that probability can
be used to make better guesses. Hilda agrees but found the
deterministic/probabilistic contrast confusing. The random walk idea went over
her head but she was pleased when Feynman acknowledged his own uncertainty
“when he states that his theory can change with future knowledge.”
For
Yikal, Feynman’s simple questions invoking probability were the lecture’s most
memorable features. “What is the chance
of rain for today? This is basically asking, what is the probability that it
will rain today? This helps us see whether we should take an umbrella or not. If
the probability is too low, then umbrella won’t be necessary. Feynman’s
conclusion: almost every choice we make is based on probability.” Also, “we can
never be 100% certain that something will happen. And sometimes we know that
something will happen but we just don’t know when it will happen. Every choice
we make is based on the probability of the benefits and the chances that
something good could come out of that system. For instance we are not 100% sure
that we will get a good job based on our career but we go to school to be
educated because there is a good chance of getting the job if we have degree.”
Kerlyn found Feynman’s focus on
the connection between chance, different types of probability and nature most
fascinating. She had vaguely heard about the Heisenberg uncertainty principle
before but explained in the context of probability made the principle real for
her. “If we try to ‘pin down’ a particle to a specific place, it will
go faster. But if it is forced to go slow, it will spread out. Our most precise
description of nature is in terms of probabilities.”
Kyle
summarizes his understanding of Feynman’s lecture by quoting from it: “There are many different types of
probability, such as independent, mutually exclusive, non-mutually exclusive, conditional
probability and inverse probability. The uncertainty principle describes an
inherent fuzziness that must exist in any attempt to describe nature. Our most
precise description of nature must be in terms of probabilities.
In the early days of the development of quantum mechanics, Einstein was quite
worried about this problem. He used to shake his head and say, ‘But surely God
does not throw dice in determining how electrons should go!’ He worried about
that problem for a long time and he probably never really reconciled himself to
the fact that this is the best description of nature that one can give. There
are still one or two physicists who are working on the problem who have an
intuitive conviction that it is possible somehow to describe the world in a
different way and that all of this uncertainty about the way things are can be
removed. No one has yet been successful.” For both Kerlyn and Kyle, this means
that the last word on the subject is perhaps yet to be written, which is what
makes the quest for knowledge so profoundly satisfying.
Jennifer found the connection
between probability and chemistry in Feynman’s lecture compelling. She also
made connection with she learned in her statistics class, that “regarding
probability density, the area under the curve, known as the bell-curve, is
equal to 1. Standard deviation is the variation from the mean.” To visually
imagine standard deviation, Feynman illustrates the motion of a molecule. He
describes an occurrence when ‘an organic compound’ is released from a bottle in
a room. This organic compound then evaporates in the air, and the particles
spread throughout, thus resulting in standard deviation.
For Aisa,
a clear, declarative sentence like, “There are good guesses and there are bad
guesses. The theory of probability is a system for making better guesses,” is
as powerful an introduction to probability as anyone can think of. She finds
Feynman’s ability to place probability in a unique perspective the main draw of
the lecture. “It makes readers think of probability not just as a sort of math
problem but something that happens in the real world. Feynman puts thinking and
logic into a different realm, and that applies to his lecture on probability as
well. He shows how probability is subjective. The answer may not always be what
you hope for or want. Still, it is better to be probabilistic and realize that
probability is a game of chances. I think this type of mind frame will help
people think of probability in a different way.”
Sabrina’s understanding of probability grew when she worked through Feynman’s
explanation of the binomial probability by breaking down the outcomes of
flipping a coin and identifying some of the rules of the binomial model, such
as, the observations must be repeatable, and the repeated observations must be
equivalent. “He makes it clear that the observations are estimates of
what will occur. The same reasoning can be generalized to any situations where
there are different, but equally likely possible results of an observation. This
of course makes perfect sense especially keeping randomness in mind. Feynman
includes a fascinating graph that represents the idea that with an increase of
number of tosses, the closer ‘the tendency is for the fraction of heads to
approach 0.5, as compared to a smaller number of tosses where the fluctuation
of deviation might be greater.’ Feynman then connects the ideas of the coin
toss to random walk and motions of atoms in a gas. This is what I found most
fascinating: How Feynman can take a simple concept and connect it to something
like the motions of atoms in a gas. We should see more connections in our
studies, whether within disciplines or between disciplines. That will motivate
students far more than treating subjects as if they were disconnected from each
other.”
No comments:
Post a Comment